Home Store Project Ideas Project Guide Ask An Expert Blog Careers Teachers Parents Students

Are Your Eyes Playing Tricks on You? Discover the Science Behind Afterimages!

Difficulty
Time Required Short (2-5 days)
Prerequisites A computer with Internet access is recommended.
Material Availability Readily available
Cost Very Low (under $20)
Safety No issues

Abstract

Yogi Berra said "You can observe a lot by just watching." In this human biology science fair project, you will observe how your eyes perceive color by watching afterimages. Afterimages are the images you see after staring at an object for several seconds and then looking away. You will also learn how different cone cells in your retina respond to different colors.

Objective

Investigate color perception by observing afterimages, which are the images left on your retina after you stare at an object and then look away.

Credits

David Whyte, PhD, Science Buddies

Cite This Page

MLA Style

Science Buddies Staff. "Are Your Eyes Playing Tricks on You? Discover the Science Behind Afterimages!" Science Buddies. Science Buddies, 14 Oct. 2013. Web. 30 Sep. 2014 <http://www.sciencebuddies.org/science-fair-projects/project_ideas/HumBio_p021.shtml>

APA Style

Science Buddies Staff. (2013, October 14). Are Your Eyes Playing Tricks on You? Discover the Science Behind Afterimages!. Retrieved September 30, 2014 from http://www.sciencebuddies.org/science-fair-projects/project_ideas/HumBio_p021.shtml

Share your story with Science Buddies!

I did this project I Did This Project! Please log in and let us know how things went.


Last edit date: 2013-10-14

Introduction

We perceive color in the world around us with special sensory cells, called cone cells. Cone cells function in bright light and are responsible for color vision and high-acuity vision, which allows you to see fine details. Cone cells reside in the region at the back of the eye called the fovea. The fovea is quite small, about 1 square millimeter (mm). It is amazing to think that color and detail we see depends on such a small bit of tissue!

There are three types of cone cells, loosely called blue, green, and red, depending on which type of light they respond to best. When you look at a red object, for example, the red cone cells are stimulated. The stimulation of the red cone cells causes them to send a message to the brain, basically saying, "the color of the object is red."

When blue light and red light are added, it creates the color purple. So when you see the color purple, your blue and your red cone cells are both stimulated.

What about white? If you have seen a rainbow, or the colors produced when sunlight passes through a prism, you have seen for yourself that white light can be separated into different colors. Adding blue, green, and red light together produces white light. If you take three flashlights and put red, blue, and green filters on them, and shine them all on the same spot, the combined colors produce white light. Any two colors whose light together produces white are called complementary colors. Examples are red and green, blue and orange, yellow and purple. Note that one color in the combination is a primary color, while the other is a mix of the other two primary colors. When you look at a white image, all three types of cones are stimulated. In other words, the brain interprets the combined input from all three types of cones and tells you that "the color is white."

Looking at a particular color for very long can cause cone cell fatigue—the cone cells get tired. For example, if you stare at a red object, the red cone cells become fatigued and are temporarily unable to respond. If you look at a white area after staring at a red image, you will see an image that is the same size and shape as the original image, but a different color. The image you see on the white surface is called an afterimage. You can see an afterimage by looking at the red circle in Figure 1, in the Experimental Procedure, for 30 seconds, then looking at the white region in the square to the right. Can you see the blue-green afterimage?

Why is the afterimage blue-green? When you look at the white surface after staring at the red-colored object, your eyes will essentially receive equal doses of red, green, and blue light coming into your eyes, but only the blue and the green cones are able to respond and tell your brain what you are seeing. Because of cone cell fatigue, the input from the red cones is missing from the region of the afterimage. So you see the afterimage as blue-green. (Here is a gross fact! Surgeons wear blue-green scrubs to minimize the afterimage of blood!)

The afterimage disappears after several seconds because the red cone cells recover from their fatigue and become active again. When the red cone cells are active again, all three types of cone cells respond to the white surface, and you see the white region normally. You can even predict the color of the afterimage for most colors—it is the complementary color to the color of the object.

In this human biology science fair project, you will investigate how your cone cells respond to various colors by observing afterimages.

Terms and Concepts

  • Cone cell
  • High-acuity vision
  • Fovea
  • Stimulation
  • Complementary colors
  • Primary colors
  • Cone cell fatigue
  • Afterimage
  • Retina

Questions

  • How many cone cells are there in 1 square mm in the fovea? How many cone cells are in 1 square mm at the edges of the retina?
  • How are cone cells spread over the retina?
  • Cone cells are sometimes described as being sensitive to short-, medium-, or long-wavelength light. Which wavelengths correspond to red, green, and blue light?
  • What causes color blindness?
  • What parts of the brain receive signals from the cone cells?

Bibliography

The University of Sussex has a website packed with information about the eye:

This website, sponsored by the Shimojo Laboratory at the California Institute of Technology, has some nice afterimage information:

This site, from the University of Washington, has a wealth of information about neuroscience, the eye, and afterimages:

The Exploratorium has a page with more project ideas about afterimages:

A visual illusion based on cone cell fatigue won a contest by the Neural Correlate Society for "The Best Visual Illusion of 2008." You can see the illusion here:

Materials and Equipment

  • Computer with a color monitor, or you can print the figures out on a color printer
  • Stopwatch or a clock or watch with a second hand
  • Helper
  • Lab notebook
  • Colored pencils or a basic computer graphics program

Share your story with Science Buddies!

I did this project I Did This Project! Please log in and let us know how things went.

Experimental Procedure

There are three parts. Each part should take less than 1 hour.

In Part 1, you will stare at a colored object for various amounts of time, then measure how long it takes for the afterimage to disappear each time. You will then graph the data as Cone stimulation (how long you stared at the colored object) vs. Afterimage persistence (how long before the afterimage disappears). Things to think about: Is there a minimum cone-stimulation time required to produce an afterimage? Do longer cone-stimulation times always cause longer afterimage times, or does the effect level out past a certain time?

In Part 2, you will test whether or not afterimages made by three different colors disappear at the same rate (this measures cone cell recovery).

In Part 3, you will test which color produces an afterimage in the least amount of time (this measures the rate of cone cell fatigue).

Part 1: Duration, Shape, and Color of an Afterimage

  1. Look at Figure 1, below. Have your helper get ready to time you for exactly 30 seconds. You should say "start" when you begin looking at the object, and the helper can say "stop" when 30 seconds have elapsed.
  2. Stare at the red circle for 30 seconds. Try not to blink. When 30 seconds are up, look at the white space in the square to the right. Describe the size, shape, and color of the afterimage in your lab notebook. Note: It is important to view this on a bright monitor with clear colors, or if you printed it out, that the color is vivid.
  3. Turn off any lights near your computer or the printed-out image. Stare at the red circle for 30 seconds again, but this time, record how long the afterimage is visible in your lab notebook. Have your helper use the stopwatch to keep track of how long you are able to see the afterimage. You might want to say "start" as soon as you begin looking at the white space and keep saying "I still see it" repeatedly until it disappears, then say "it's gone" so your helper can stop the stopwatch and you can note the time. Why does the afterimage disappear?
  4. Repeat step 3 two more times, for a total of three trials, and record all data in a data table in your lab notebook, like Table 1, below. Note: Give your eyes a minute or so to rest between every trial.
    Human Biology  Science Project red circle figure
    Figure 1. This is the first afterimage test for this science fair project. View the red circle on the left for 30 seconds, and then note the afterimage when you look in the white space to the right.
    Now let's see how changing the amount of time you look at the red circle ("cone stimulation" time) changes the time that the afterimage is visible ("afterimage persistence"). How do you think the length of time the afterimage persists will vary?
  5. Repeat steps 3-4 four more times, changing the amount of time you look at the red circle each time. Look at the red circle for 5, 10, 20, and then 60 seconds before you look at the white area (you already completed the 30-second trial in steps 3-4).
  6. Record how long the afterimage persists after each time in your data table.

     Afterimage Persistence (seconds)
    Cone Stimulation
    (seconds)
    Red Circle
    Trial 1
    Red Circle
    Trial 2
    Red Circle
    Trial 3
    Red Circle
    Average
    5    
    10    
    20    
    30    
    60    
    Table 1. In your lab notebook, record your data in a data table like this one.

  7. Calculate the averages and insert that data in the last column.
  8. Graph the data. Put the number of seconds you looked at the object on the x-axis and the average time the afterimage persisted on the y-axis. Call the x-axis Cone Stimulation and the y-axis Afterimage Persistence, or choose your own axis names. Include the units (seconds) on the graph.
  9. What was the maximum amount of time that the afterimage persisted?
  10. If you stare at the object for longer periods of time, beyond 30 seconds (say 60, 90, and 120 seconds) does the persistence time of the afterimage also keep increasing? Explain your results in terms of the level of fatigue of the cone cells.

Part 2: Determining Cone Cell Recovery Rates

Human Biology  Science Project three-color circle figure
Figure 2. Use this three-colored circle to compare cone cell fatigue and recovery times.
  1. Turn off any lights near your computer or printed-out image. Stare at the three-colored circle in Figure 2 for 30 seconds and then look at the white space to the right. Using the same method as in Part 1, have your helper time how long you see the afterimage and record the data in your lab notebook.
  2. Also describe the colors of the afterimage in your lab notebook. In your descriptions, call the three sectors Left, Right, and Bottom. This avoids confusion about the colors.
  3. Use colored pencils or a computer graphics program to draw the afterimage.
  4. Repeat Part 2 steps 1-3 two more times, for a total of three trials, recording your data each time. And don't forget to give your eyes a break between each trial.
  5. How do afterimage persistence times compare for the blue, red, and green regions? Do the three sectors disappear at the same rate, or at different rates?

Part 3: Determining Cone Cell Fatigue Rate

  1. Look at the three-colored circle in Figure 2 very briefly, for 2 or 3 seconds, then look at the white space. Record what you see or don't see in your lab notebook.
  2. Repeat Part 3 step 1 with different times (1, 2, 3, 4 seconds) to determine the minimum time you need to form an afterimage.
  3. Did the afterimage include all three segments? Or did only one of the colors make an afterimage? If so, how would you interpret this in terms of which cone cell fatigued the fastest?
  4. Record your observations in your lab notebook.

Share your story with Science Buddies!

I did this project I Did This Project! Please log in and let us know how things went.


Variations

  • What happens if you keep one eye closed? For example, look at the red circle in Figure 1 with just your left eye. Then look at the white region with either your left eye or your right eye. What do you predict will happen?
  • Do different people have the same afterimage results? Repeat the afterimage experiments with several (at least three) friends and relatives.
  • Make colored objects of your own, and predict what the color of the afterimage will be. Make a table with the color of the object and the predicted color of the afterimage (remember that the color of the afterimage will be complementary to the color of the object).
  • You can add some "wildlife" to this project by designing animal shapes as your colored objects, and cages or aquariums in the area of the afterimage. Visit the Exploratorium Museum in San Francisco, California's website for more ideas.

Share your story with Science Buddies!

I did this project I Did This Project! Please log in and let us know how things went.

Ask an Expert

The Ask an Expert Forum is intended to be a place where students can go to find answers to science questions that they have been unable to find using other resources. If you have specific questions about your science fair project or science fair, our team of volunteer scientists can help. Our Experts won't do the work for you, but they will make suggestions, offer guidance, and help you troubleshoot.

Ask an Expert

Related Links

If you like this project, you might enjoy exploring these related careers:

optometrist conducting eye exam

Optometrist

Optometrists are the primary caretakers of our most important sense—vision. They diagnose and detect problems not only with vision, but with the health of the eye and the whole body. Based on their diagnoses, they prescribe glasses, contact lenses, and medications; refer patients to opthamologists for surgery; or develop treatment plans, like vision therapy, to help correct for deficits in depth perception. Their work helps people live better at every stage of life. Read more
Neurologist adjusting settings on Parkinson patient's neurostimulator

Neurologist

Each time your heart beats, or you breathe, think, dream, smell, see, move, laugh, read, remember, write, or feel something, you are using your nervous system. The nervous system includes your brain, spinal cord, and a huge network of nerves that make electrical connections all over your body. Neurologists are the medical doctors who diagnose and treat problems with the nervous system. They work to restore health to an essential system in the body. Read more

Looking for more science fun?

Try one of our science activities for quick, anytime science explorations. The perfect thing to liven up a rainy day, school vacation, or moment of boredom.

Find an Activity